Skip to main content

The spacing effect: little and often

Research - what there is of it - shows that humans tend to retain information better when they learn in short bursts at intervals rather in one big chunk. To be anecdotal, I find that when I am learning a song for my barbershop chorus I learn more effectively if I listen to my recordings for a short while at a time, leaving at least a day between each learning session. If I want to improve my stick skills on the drums I get more success practising ten minutes each day, rather than 70 minutes in one weekly session. I wonder what your experience is?

I thought about this after reading a post on the TES MFL forum from a teacher who is to be lumbered next year with two hour lessons for languages. Two hours!

My own hunch about this is that it is better to have four shorter sessions of language learning a week, rather than two long ones. These days many schools use one hour slots which means that students can only have two or three language sessions a week. I believe some schools like 70 minute lessons, while others use even longer slots. At Ripon Grammar School we had a 40 x 40 minute timetable, which meant that at KS3 we had four or even five language lessons per week (one language).

Shorter lessons encourage the teacher to work at pace, they allow for considerable target language input nearly every day and ensure that pupils are less likely to get bored. The spaced learning effect should lead to better retention and acquisition. A further advantage is that, if school is disrupted by a special event on one day, you lose less time.

Are there any downsides? Well, you might argue that a little time is lost in moving around school, but the considerable number of double lessons in a 40 x 40 minute means this is not a great issue. Short lessons can make it harder to plan for assessments. Some activities may benefit from a longer time in one chunk. Compared with the benefits of "little and often", these seem to me to be side issues.

The surprisingly little research done on this supports the idea that "spaced learning" is more effective. In other words, the "little and often" principle is sound. When one bears this in mind, along with the (no doubt related) fact that children lose concentration quickly I question how effective timetabling is for languages. One advantage of operating with shorter periods of 35 or 40 minutes is that you can offer double periods for practical subjects which require more time and shorter periods for subjects where repeated practice is more successful (languages and, I surmise, maths for example).

For the theory behind this:

http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Spacing_effect

Timetabling is a fundamental part of schooling. At the moment linguists have to make do with a "one-size-fits- all" policy which works against pupils who already are handicapped by having far too little time overall as it is.

Comments

  1. Our students mainly have one two-hour session per week of MFL. There is a considerable benefit to this model in terms of managing behaviour in the corridors. However, those classes who have two one-hour sessions do seem to make better progress. I'd be interested to see how it might work with 30 minute lessons, though it sounds more difficult to plan for...

    ReplyDelete
  2. Thanks for commenting. I never found planning a problem with 40 minute lessons. You do have to be flexible if some things go quicker or slower than expected, but this would be the case with any system. With 4 x 30 minutes you might be able to sustain 10 minutes of solid oral work per lesson i.e. 40 minutes in total. Would you do 40 minutes oral work in a 2 hour lesson? I doubt it.

    ReplyDelete

Post a Comment

Popular posts from this blog

What is the natural order hypothesis?

The natural order hypothesis states that all learners acquire the grammatical structures of a language in roughly the same order. This applies to both first and second language acquisition. This order is not dependent on the ease with which a particular language feature can be taught; in English, some features, such as third-person "-s" ("he runs") are easy to teach in a classroom setting, but are not typically fully acquired until the later stages of language acquisition. The hypothesis was based on morpheme studies by Heidi Dulay and Marina Burt, which found that certain morphemes were predictably learned before others during the course of second language acquisition. The hypothesis was picked up by Stephen Krashen who incorporated it in his very well known input model of second language learning. Furthermore, according to the natural order hypothesis, the order of acquisition remains the same regardless of the teacher's explicit instruction; in other words,

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a language).

12 principles of second language teaching

This is a short, adapted extract from our book The Language Teacher Toolkit . "We could not possibly recommend a single overall method for second language teaching, but the growing body of research we now have points to certain provisional broad principles which might guide teachers. Canadian professors Patsy Lightbown and Nina Spada (2013), after reviewing a number of studies over the years to see whether it is better to just use meaning-based approaches or to include elements of explicit grammar teaching and practice, conclude: Classroom data from a number of studies offer support for the view that form-focused instruction and corrective feedback provided within the context of communicative and content-based programmes are more effective in promoting second language learning than programmes that are limited to a virtually exclusive emphasis on comprehension. As teachers Gianfranco and I would go along with that general view and would like to suggest our own set of g