Skip to main content

Semantic versus thematic clustering of vocabulary

This is a snippet from our forthcoming book about memory. This is from a chapter about remembering vocabulary.

 

Researchers have sought more efficient ways of learning vocabulary from lists. One popular comparison has been made between semantic and thematic vocabulary clustering types. Semantic clusters provide students with groups of words that are related by their meanings. For example, parts of the body, such as eye, head, ear and mouth. The argument for semantic clusters is appealing. Firstly, the similarity between the words should ease the learning task and secondly, the student should become aware of slight distinctions between the related words. In addition, most of us have been used to learning and teaching words in this way.

Nation (2001) argues that:

1.      - It requires less effort to learn words in a set.

2.      - It is easier to retrieve related words from memory.

3.      - It helps learners see how knowledge can be organised.

4.      - It reflects the way such information is stored in the brain (so-called semantic fields).

5.   - It makes the meaning of words clearer by helping students to see how they relate to and may be differentiated from other words in the set.

But the downside of teaching words in this way is that words of similar meaning may cause interference effects in memory. The closer two words are in meaning or association (including synonyms and antonyms), the greater the risk of interference and forgetting.

On the other hand, thematic clusters refer to the arrangement of a group of words that belong to a specific knowledge schema. The advantage is thought to be that memory is activated more powerfully when words are related to lived experience or episodes (knowledge schemas). So if you teach a group of words in the context of a lived experience the words should be easier to recall later. Tinkham (1997) suggested that arranging words by general theme in this way can limit the effects of interference between similar words. An example of a thematic cluster would be sweat-shirt, changing room, tries on, wool and salesperson.

So what is the evidence? On the whole, researchers now favour thematic word sets to semantic. Some studies report that semantic grouping is actually worse than presenting lists of totally unrelated words. So if your textbook presents words as semantic clusters you should at least question the validity of this approach, which may just stem from tradition. As Dronjic (2019) points out, thematic clustering is better on the whole than semantic clustering and better also than just listing words randomly.

Does this mean you should stop playing Simon Says to teach parts of the body? Not at all. Don’t forget the importance of motivation, distinctiveness and gesture in forming memories! In any case, all researchers agree that learning from lists, although apparently efficient, is a very small part of what learning vocabulary is all about.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a langua...

Zaz - Si jamais j'oublie

My wife and I often listen to Radio Paradise, a listener-supported, ad-free radio station from California. They've been playing this song by Zaz recently. I like it and maybe your students would too. I shouldn't really  reproduce the lyrics here for copyright reasons, but I am going to translate them (with the help of another video). You could copy and paste this translation and set it for classwork (not homework, I suggest, since students could just go and find the lyrics online). The song was released in 2015 and gotr to number 11 in the French charts - only number 11! Here we go: Remind me of the day and the year Remind me of the weather And if I've forgotten, you can shake me And if I want to take myself away Lock me up and throw away the key With pricks of memory Tell me what my name is If I ever forget the nights I spent, the guitars, the cries Remind me who I am, why I am alive If I ever forget, if I ever take to my heels If one day I run away Remind me who I am, wha...

What is the phonological loop?

This post is about how we use part of our short-term memory (working memory) to process sounds, words and longer utterances. I also intend to show how knowing about the phonological loop can help you refine your practice as a language teacher. Firstly, what is the phonological loop and where does it fit into a popular model of working memory? To start with, it's probably best to start by activating another component of short-term memory, your visuo-spatial sketchpad. Look at this diagram: Image from cheese360 at Wikimedia Commons That is one depiction of the well-known model of working memory put forward by cognitive psychologists Alan Baddeley and Graham Hitch back in 1974. But first, when we see, hear, touch, taste or smell something our sensory memory takes note (beneath our consciousness). As far as language is concerned, we choose to pay attention to it and the information enters working memory, more specifically what are called the visuo-spatial sketchpad (aka scr...