Skip to main content

Identifying and teaching high-attainers

This post aims to give you a little background about the topic of second language learning aptitude, how to identify high aptitude language learners and what you might do with this information. It is based on a section of Memory: What Every Language Teacher Should Know (Smith and Conti, 2021).

Teachers sometimes hesitate before using the phrases high ability and low ability since they risk implying that a student's attainment is constrained by a pre-existing level of aptitude. However, few if any teachers would not recognise that some students just seem to be far quicker than others. There is, in fact, quite a long tradition of research into language learning aptitude.

There’s no doubt that aptitude for language learning exists and back in 1959 John Carroll and Stanley Sapon attempted to identify the factors which make up aptitude and predict a person’s ability to learn another language (Carroll and Sapon, 1959). Carroll is considered by many to be one of the premier psychologists in terms of contributions to educational linguistics. The audio and pen and paper tests they designed (called the MLAT – Modern Language Aptitude Test) are still used today, as well as later variations. Proficiency is, of course, also a product of acquired knowledge, not just aptitude. The aptitude test shows potential, not achievement. Even if a learner has lower aptitude, this does not imply some sort of cap on eventual achievement.

Below are the factors Carroll identified and which you can use to identify higher aptitude learners.

1. Phonetic coding ability - the ability to identify distinct sounds, to form associations between those sounds and symbols representing them, and to retain these associations.

2. Grammatical sensitivity — the ability to recognise the grammatical functions of words (or other linguistic entities) in sentence structures. 

3. Rote learning ability for second language materials — the ability to learn associations between sounds and meanings rapidly and efficiently, and to retain these associations.

4. Inductive language learning ability — the ability to infer or induce the rules governing a set of language materials, given samples of language materials that permit such inferences. 

Although working memory (short-term memory) was not particularly on the agenda in 1959, you can see how it is involved in all of the above factors. A few researchers, e.g. Wen (2016) even hypothesise that working memory is the key element of aptitude. Students have the above characteristics to varying degrees. You have encountered the good mimic who is not so good at grammar, and the accurate, grammatically aware writer who has more difficulty pronouncing accurately or speaking fluently.

Peter Skehan’s 2002 model of language aptitude and four stages in the acquisition process is summarised in the table below (adapted from Ellis and Shintani, 2013). He combines the notion of aptitude with cognitive processes involved in information processing models of learning (encoding, storing and retrieving via working memory and long-term memory).

Stage

Processes involved

Aptitude components

Noticing

The student directs attention to a feature in the input

Auditory segmentation; attention management; working memory; phonemic coding

Patterning

The student forms a hypothesis (explicitly or implicitly) about the feature, then extends the range of the hypothesis before recognising its limitations, restructuring it and integrating the new representation into the developing language system

Working memory; grammatical sensitivity; inductive language learning ability; restructuring capacity

Controlling

The student can use the integrated feature with increasing ease and accuracy

Automatisation; proceduralisation; retrieval processes

Lexicalising

The student can now produce the feature as a remembered whole rather than by applying a rule

Long-term memory; chunking; retrieval processes

 

On the whole, you'll recognise those high attainers in your classroom, although some are good at hiding their light under a bushel! They appear attentive, concentrate well, mimic accurately, pick up patterns and can hold longer sentences in mind, uttering them fluently. They find it easier to say longer words and can discriminate more easily between unfamiliar phonemes. In Skehan's terms, they notice, pattern, control and lexicalise more easily.

Once we know something about aptitude, how can be use this information be used to benefit the highest attainers – often the students who will go to study languages to a more advanced level? Below are some factors to bear in mind when curriculum planning.

        ·  The provision of open-ended oral and written tasks. Does the   curriculum specifically include such tasks with the aim of  stretching the most able?
        ·  Classroom techniques designed to match questions to      students (as opposed to random “cold-call” questioning). Are  all teachers versed in skilled questioning technique?
        ·  Opportunities for extra listening and reading input. Does the  department have the necessary sources available?
        ·   A consideration of class grouping by aptitude or prior achievement. If the school culture supports ability groups, what would be the best format and how would you assign teachers to each group for the benefit of all students?
        ·     Differentiated tasks, for example, varying the length of oral presentations or the titles of written essays. How could this be incorporated while not sacrificing a “mastery for all” approach?
  • Provision for high-aptitude linguists who may have conditions such as ADHD or dyslexia. Can you identify them (for example, the good mimic who may produce inaccurate written work)? Can you adjust teaching and resources to cater for their needs?
From my own experience I would pick out the advantage of having a high ability group working together. This obviously comes at a potential cost (I won't go into the social equity arguments here), but if your school's culture and timetabling allow for setting by ability or prior achievement (the two go hand in hand to a good extent), having a 'top set' allows you to build up a real momentum, matching input and practice type to ability, and accelerating progress. Schools wishing to increase uptake at A-level in England, wales and Northern Ireland may like to explore this if they are not already using sets. Alas, there is scant research into this when it comes specifically to language learning.

References

Carroll, J. B., & Sapon, S. M. (1959). Modern language aptitude test. Psychological Corporation.
Ellis, R. and Shintani, N.  (2013) Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research. London: Routledge.
Smith, S.P. and Conti, G. (2021). Memory: What Every Language Teacher Should Know. Independently published.
Wen, Z. (2016). Working Memory and Second Language Learning. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a language).

The 2026 GCSE subject content is published!

Two DfE documents were published today. The first was the response to the consultation about the proposed new GCSE (originally due in October 2021) and the second is the subject content document which, ultimately, is of most interest to MFL teachers in England. Here is the link  to the document.  We are talking about an exam to be done from 2026 (current Y7s). There is always a tendency for sceptical teachers to think that consultations are a bit of a sham and that the DfE will just go ahead and do what they want when it comes to exam reform. In this case, the responses to the original proposals were mixed, and most certainly hostile as far as exam boards and professional associations representing the MFL community, universities, head teachers and awarding bodies are concerned. What has emerged does reveal some significant changes which take account of a number of criticisms levelled at the proposals. As I read it, the most important changes relate to vocabulary and the issue of topics

La retraite à 60 ans

Suite à mon post récent sur les acquis sociaux..... L'âge légal de la retraite est une chose. Je voudrais bien savoir à quel âge les gens prennent leur retraite en pratique - l'âge réel de la retraite, si vous voulez. J'ai entendu prétendre qu'il y a peu de différence à cet égard entre la France et le Royaume-Uni. Manifestation à Marseille en 2008 pour le maintien de la retraite à 60 ans © AFP/Michel Gangne Six Français sur dix sont d’accord avec le PS qui défend la retraite à 60 ans (BVA) Cécile Quéguiner Plus de la moitié des Français jugent que le gouvernement a " tort de vouloir aller vite dans la réforme " et estiment que le PS a " raison de défendre l’âge légal de départ en retraite à 60 ans ". Résultat d’un sondage BVA/Absoluce pour Les Échos et France Info , paru ce matin. Une majorité de Français (58%) estiment que la position du Parti socialiste , qui défend le maintien de l’âge légal de départ à la retraite à 60 ans,