This is an adapted extract from our book Memory: What Every Language Teacher Should Know (2021). It's about the the concept of phonological memory.
In
the multi-store model of working memory shown above, based on the Alan Baddeley model, (figure from Smith and Conti, 2021), the phonological
loop is the most important part as far as language
learning is concerned. How does it
work? Think for a moment about how you hold a word or
phrase in your head as you make sense of it, or prepare to say it; or how you
say words in your head as you read from a book; or how you try to make sense of
some spoken language. These processes are carried out by the phonological loop,
what psychologists sometimes call verbal or phonological working memory.
The phonological loop has been
described as the mind’s ear, processing and storing sounds. You could think of it as our
language learning device, activated in a particular part of the brain (the
temporal lobe of the left hemisphere - above and around your left ear). In the common Baddeley model of working memory, the phonological loop consists of
two components:
·
The phonological
store, which has a limited capacity and holds acoustic information
briefly.
·
The articulatory
rehearsal process, which allows us to mentally rehearse language to
prevent it from decaying, e.g. when you repeat a sentence in your head a few
times.
The phonological loop interacts with
long-term memory and plays a vital role in the long term recall of the
phonological form (sound) of new words and phrases. As new phonological forms are held
in the phonological store during rehearsal, so more permanent memory
representations are constructed. This is one reason why it’s so important to
allow students to hear and repeat new language as often as possible. They need
to have a phonological representation of words, not just know what they mean or
look like. This makes it easier for them to recognise words and chunks in the
continuous stream of speech.
We know some other significant things about the phonological loop. Its capacity or span is as few as three to five items of language at the same time: but recall that these can be sounds, words, phrases or even whole sentences. This capacity varies to a limited degree from person to person, has a genetic basis and declines as we age after our teens.
The
limited capacity of the phonological loop means that a novice second language
learner can hold fewer words in working memory than in their first language as
they pronounce the words more slowly. The more rapidly a second language
speaker can utter a word or phrase, the less space it takes in working memory.
So the more you know, and the more fluently you can speak, the easier the job
becomes for working memory. This means that we need to carefully control
the amount and difficulty level of language students hear and read.
Furthermore,
research shows that learners can recall pairs of words which sound the same
less well than those which sound different. So for example, a students might
find it easier to remember the pair of French words violon and trompette than
the pair fraise (strawberry) and frais (fresh). This is called the phonological similarity effect.
Research suggests that the negative interference caused by this effect on
memory is stronger than the one caused by the orthographic similarity
effect (when two words have similar spellings). Often, though, the two effects
combine to make it harder to learn words which sound similar.
This
phenomenon has important implications for learning and helps us understand why
learning verb conjugations in highly inflected languages (e.g. French and
Spanish) can be so challenging, especially for students with a less efficient
working memory. In the case of French verbs, the phonological similarity effect
is particularly strong, as many verb forms are pronounced exactly the same
(e.g. je mange, tu manges il mange, elles mangent). Not
surprisingly, many learners of French find it hard to map the phonological form
these verbs to the correct spelling.
To deal with this, we can do a range of phonics (sound to spelling) activities, focused on verb forms (e.g. partial dictations in which the verb endings are gapped, crossing out silent letters, correcting faulty transcripts, and so on). Input-enhancement techniques can also be used, for instance using a specific colour for each problematic verb form and its associated pronoun (e.g. in French je mange, tu manges, il/elle mange, ils/elles mangent). These provide students with an extra retrieval cue to help them remember.
Next, a word about quiet reading... As
you read silently to yourself you cannot resist saying the words in your head.
Try it now! When we see written language, we simultaneously
hear it. By combining what we see with its mental, phonological activation we
engage both visual and phonological processes to reinforce memory. This is an example dual coding. Quiet reading, reading while listening, or reading aloud, all help to
build memory and therefore long-term proficiency. But the quiet reading will be
far more useful if students already have a firm grasp of phonics. To put it
rather snappily, our visual field says "shapes!," while our
phonological loop says "sounds!"
When
we acquire our first language(s) we learn the acceptable sounds and sound
combinations within the first few months of life. Not surprising perhaps that
when phonotactic rules (many teachers will think of grammar rules here) for a
first and second language are the same, words are more quickly detected by the
phonological loop.
In
other words, words which are easier to pronounce are learned more quickly. This
means that cognates (words which mean, sound and look the same across two
languages) are more easily remembered, when spoken, than other words, as the
phonological loop recognises their similarity with the first language
phonology.
Since
words which are easier to pronounced are learned more quickly, internalising
the phonotactics of a language is important. It therefore helps to explicitly
teach the phonotactic rules of the target language and give plenty of
opportunities to use connected speech right from the start.
This helps along the process of chunking words together, speeding up both acquisition and spoken fluency. Why? Fluent speech is about sequencing meaningful chunks (a process we called chaining). With this in mind, the common practice of teaching words in isolation is not the best way to develop fluency. Better to do individual, choral and interactive reading aloud activities, structured question-answer sequences, structured drills or any task requiring the repeated use of common chunks of language. Providing large amounts of comprehensible listening and reading input, flooded with the same chunks, also contributes to memory building, speeding up language processing and developing oral fluency.
Reference
Smith, S.P. & Conti, G (2021). Memory: What Every Language Teacher Should Know. Independently published. Available from Amazon.
Comments
Post a Comment