This is another extract from The Language Teacher Toolkit (Smith and Conti, 2023), and follows on from the previous post about types of grammatical knowledge. If you want to explore more posts about grammar, have a look here, here and here.
Here is a thought-provoking post by Gianfranco Conti.
And here is a very practical post from James Stubbs, about moving students from implicit to explicit knowledge.
Here are out 10 principles
1. Do not make learning grammar rules the focus of language teaching (Ellis and Shintani, 2013). Grammar teaching can
reduce enjoyment (Graham, 2022). In particular, make the grammar content
lighter with students with lower language-analytic ability, i.e. being able to
derive rules by analysing language. Grammar teaching is more suitable for high-aptitude learners (Suzuki and DeKeyser, 2017).
2. Combine the use of explicit and implicit learning (Ellis and Shintani, 2013). To maximise implicit learning, use highly patterned input, input-flooding and input enhancement, i.e. emphasizing specific grammar items, through phonetic or visual devices (such as pronouncing a verb ending louder, highlighting a preposition, colour-coding case endings in German). Another way to maximise implicit learning is to exploit input at every level (Ellis, 2015), through intensive reading and listening.
3. Introduce new
structures in familiar linguistic contexts, e.g. with known vocabulary, to decrease
cognitive load (Smith and Conti, 2021). Practise new grammar structures in
sentences which are simple, short and where vocabulary and pronunciation create
less cognitive load.
4. Gradually phase out
scaffolds. Move gradually from receptive processing to highly controlled, then
semi-controlled and finally unplanned production. The rate at which you do this
depends on the class. Research suggests that successful acquisition of a
grammatical feature correlates with success in the initial retrieval episodes
(Boers, 2021). So, go to production when you are confident students will be
successful.
5. Be aware of factors
affecting acquisition, including issues such as L1 positive/negative transfer, saliency (how
noticeable features are), regularity, frequency, complexity, phonological
issues and so on. Anticipate these in your planning (Ellis, 2015). Above all, consider
whether the class is developmentally ready to acquire grammar features, notably
younger learners and lower-attaining students.
6. Compare with L1
structures. Especially for students with low L1 literacy, explain how you would
form and use the L1 equivalent of the target L2 structure first, then draw or elicit
a comparison from the students. Tribushinina et al. (2022) provide
evidence that this contrastive approach is effective for primary age children
with developmental language disorder.
7. Consider Transfer-Appropriate
Processing. Remember that if a structure is learnt and practised in a specific
linguistic context or through a given task, it is not easily transferred to
another. For instance, if reflexive verbs are only practised in the context of
daily routine, students find it difficult to transfer them to other contexts
(Smith and Conti, 2021). Similarly, grammar is best tested in the way it was
practised.
8. Assess grammar
uptake through
a mixture of structured assessment (e.g. gap-fills) and free-production
tasks, for example, “talk to me about last weekend” (Ellis and Shintani,
2013). Structured assessment tells us if students can do so when there are
clear prompts as to what they are required to retrieve; free-production tasks
test spontaneous grammar use, so tell us if the target structures have really been
acquired.
9. Use delayed
assessment. Assess the learning of a grammar structure not just when the
retrieval strength is high (such as right at the end of series of lessons
on it), but several weeks later too. This reveals how much has been truly
retained. Delayed tests can be impromptu and low stakes, so students are not
demoralised if they do badly.
10. Correct errors
directly and selectively. Research evidence is mixed on error correction, but generally suggests
that it can improve grammatical accuracy, both oral and written, if done
clearly, selectively, promptly and in a contextually sensitive way (e.g. Ellis
and Shintani, 2013).
Comments
Post a Comment