Skip to main content

How do you seat your classes?

Periodically on Twitter a thread begins about seating pupils in rows or around grouped tables. Such threads often associate seating arrangements with a general view of education. In essence, rows indicate a traditionalist view (teacher as “sage on stage”) while groups reflect a progressive perspective (“guide from the side”). I wonder what seating arrangements you use and why.

I tried out various seating systems during my career: rows, horse shoes and grouped tables, occasionally a combination of central horse shoe and rows to the side and behind. For at least 10 years my tiny classroom meant rows were the only solution. For many “itinerant” teachers you have no choice - you turn up and work with whatever is already in place. By the end of my career I used forward facing rows then moved tables into a horseshoe for small groups of about 15 or fewer.

The arguments for different seating patterns have been well rehearsed: better teacher and board visibility with rows, greater encouragement of peer discussion with groups and facilitation of whole group discussion with horseshoes. Research on this area is very limited and not much help. When we wrote The Language Teacher Toolkit we came across just one study which gave an advantage to rows for individual quiet study (less opportunity for distraction).

In the end, for larger groups of around 30 pupils I was most comfortable with rows. Why? I wanted pupils to see me and the board clearly and I wanted to be able to easily scan the class, left to right, front to back. Rows helped me establish good control and relationships. I was not a frequent user of group work, but did make frequent use of pair work, particularly what Doug Lemov calls “turn and talk”, where, after modelling work from the front, pupils then practised in pairs. Rows, therefore, did not prevent me moving away from teaching from the front.

Of course seating in rows does not preclude group work or “moving around the class” activities such as surveys, running dictation or “find someone who...” tasks. My A-level groups sat in horse shoes unless the group was larger than usual. This enabled my to sit, do whole group discussion and pair work.

I don’t see a practical or pedagogical advantage to grouped seating. Pair work is more efficient than group work as there is a greater need to contribute. In groups pupils have to turn to see you when you are explaining, using visuals or leading interactions from the front. I would think inattention is more likely when using grouped tables.

Finally, I don’t accept that seating patterns need reflect your general view of education as either traditional or progressive, teacher or child-centred. You can just see this as a pragmatic issue. What helps students work most effectively?


- Posted using BlogPress from my iPad

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

What is the natural order hypothesis?

The natural order hypothesis states that all learners acquire the grammatical structures of a language in roughly the same order. This applies to both first and second language acquisition. This order is not dependent on the ease with which a particular language feature can be taught; in English, some features, such as third-person "-s" ("he runs") are easy to teach in a classroom setting, but are not typically fully acquired until the later stages of language acquisition. The hypothesis was based on morpheme studies by Heidi Dulay and Marina Burt, which found that certain morphemes were predictably learned before others during the course of second language acquisition. The hypothesis was picked up by Stephen Krashen who incorporated it in his very well known input model of second language learning. Furthermore, according to the natural order hypothesis, the order of acquisition remains the same regardless of the teacher's explicit instruction; in other words,

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a language).

12 principles of second language teaching

This is a short, adapted extract from our book The Language Teacher Toolkit . "We could not possibly recommend a single overall method for second language teaching, but the growing body of research we now have points to certain provisional broad principles which might guide teachers. Canadian professors Patsy Lightbown and Nina Spada (2013), after reviewing a number of studies over the years to see whether it is better to just use meaning-based approaches or to include elements of explicit grammar teaching and practice, conclude: Classroom data from a number of studies offer support for the view that form-focused instruction and corrective feedback provided within the context of communicative and content-based programmes are more effective in promoting second language learning than programmes that are limited to a virtually exclusive emphasis on comprehension. As teachers Gianfranco and I would go along with that general view and would like to suggest our own set of g