Skip to main content

Using pictures for listening and creative storytelling

I've blogged before about the value of a picture in generating lots of interesting language in a communicative way. In the example below a picture of a car crash can be used to create a story involving the background to the event, what happened and the people involved. Possibilities are numerous and allow students to come up with their own angles. There are no right or wrong factual answers. Different time frames can be used, including future.

My approach would be to display the picture, possibly describe it, then do a teacher-led lesson where the main goal is listening to comprehensible input, but with additional focus on speaking, reading and writing. You would ask questions, elicit answers and write them up in full or partial form on the board. In this way students get to see the language they are hearing and use it to help generate further answers. Once there is a full set of notes on the board these can then be hidden or gapped (e.g. using the "disppearing text" technique, removing more and more words or chunks until the class can more or less recount the story from memory). So this allows students to recall the story again, generating more repetitions of useful language. The story can be written up at home or in class. A really good class could use similar language to produce their own versions of the story based on the picture.

I would do this with a good GCSE (intermediate level) class or even an advanced group. So here's the example:

Image: pixabay.com

The following questions are for guidance and would be adapted to whatever the class suggests. I've included a possible role play scenario below the questions.


1.         Que voyez-vous ici?

2.         Dans quel pays sommes-nous par exemple? Comment le savez-vous ?

3.         Depuis combien de temps se trouve cette voiture ici ?

4.         C’est à quelle saison ? Comment le savez-vous ?

5.         Qu’est-ce qui s’est passé ?

6.         Qui était le conducteur du véhicule ? Décrivez-le. Il était seul ?
7.         Il y avait des passagers? Qui? Décrivez -les.

8.         Comment est-ce que l’accident s’est produit ?

9.         Que faisait le conducteur au moment de l’accident ?

10     Qui était responsable de l’accident ? Pourquoi ?

11.       Qu’est-ce que le conducteur aurait pu faire pour éviter la collision ?

12.       Où est le conducteur actuellement ? Il est en quel état ?

13.       Si d’autres véhicules sont impliqués expliquez ce qui est arrivé aux autres conducteurs et   passagers.


Imaginez que vous êtes un policier arrivé sur scène. Téléphonez à votre gendarmerie pour expliquer ce que vous avez trouvé et ce que vous avez fait.


Vocabulaire utile

le volant
steering wheel
entrer en collision
to collide
heurter/tamponner
to hit, bump into
blesser
to injure
déraper
to skid
perdre le contrôle
to lose control
les urgences
emergency services
les pompiers
fire brigade





Comments

Popular posts from this blog

What is the natural order hypothesis?

The natural order hypothesis states that all learners acquire the grammatical structures of a language in roughly the same order. This applies to both first and second language acquisition. This order is not dependent on the ease with which a particular language feature can be taught; in English, some features, such as third-person "-s" ("he runs") are easy to teach in a classroom setting, but are not typically fully acquired until the later stages of language acquisition. The hypothesis was based on morpheme studies by Heidi Dulay and Marina Burt, which found that certain morphemes were predictably learned before others during the course of second language acquisition. The hypothesis was picked up by Stephen Krashen who incorporated it in his very well known input model of second language learning. Furthermore, according to the natural order hypothesis, the order of acquisition remains the same regardless of the teacher's explicit instruction; in other words,

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a language).

12 principles of second language teaching

This is a short, adapted extract from our book The Language Teacher Toolkit . "We could not possibly recommend a single overall method for second language teaching, but the growing body of research we now have points to certain provisional broad principles which might guide teachers. Canadian professors Patsy Lightbown and Nina Spada (2013), after reviewing a number of studies over the years to see whether it is better to just use meaning-based approaches or to include elements of explicit grammar teaching and practice, conclude: Classroom data from a number of studies offer support for the view that form-focused instruction and corrective feedback provided within the context of communicative and content-based programmes are more effective in promoting second language learning than programmes that are limited to a virtually exclusive emphasis on comprehension. As teachers Gianfranco and I would go along with that general view and would like to suggest our own set of g