Skip to main content

Retrieval practice and stress

Back from our winter sun break in Saint Lucia and Martinique, it's blog time again! By the way, francophiles out there could easily fall in love with Martinique, especially during a European winter. It's really France with rum, rum punch, coconuts, banana plantations, sugar cane and more rum. Wikipedia lists 18 rum distilleries on Martinique, not bad going for a population of around 375 000 people.

Anyway, on my return I starting looking at my recently purchased copy of The Science of Learning: 77 Studies that Every Teacher Needs to Know (2019) by Bradley Busch and Edward Watson, published by Routledge. The writers work for InnerDrive, a company which on its website states is "a mindset coaching company working in education, business and sport. We specialise in realising the potential behind Growth Mindset, Metacognition and stress management strategies and as a result improve motivation, learning, confidence and performance in our clients."

Whilst I can't vouch for the academic credentials of the writers, they have produced an attractive, easy to digest volume which consists of brief summaries of 77 key research studies from psychology, followed by advice for teachers and students based on the findings. A testimonial by Dylan Wiliam reads:

"This is the book I have been waiting for. Whether you are just beginning as a teacher, or a seasoned veteran, there will be something here that will be directly relevant to your practice, and, perhaps more importantly, will make you think."

I think he's right.

I'm going to share one research study practice, one about retrieval practice and stress. It's number 76 of the 77.

It's based on a study by Smith et al (2016) in Science, 354 (6315), 1046-1048. The article was called Retrieval practice protects memory against acute stress.

The question is this: are students who revise using retrieval practice (aka the testing effect) better able to recall information when they are stressed, for example during high stakes exams? The researchers had half their sample of students revise by doing lots of tests and quizzes (retrieval practice) and the other half by doing re-reading of key passages of text. They then placed half of each group in either stressful or non-stressful environments and recorded how much they could remember.

This is what they found.

1. The learners using retrieval practice outperformed the other group by 17-26%.

2. Increased stress made those who studied by re-reading perform 32% worse.

3. Increased stress did not adversely affect the memory of the retrieval practice group.

4. Retrieval practice was so effective in inhibiting the effects of stress on memory, that this group even outperformed the group who learned by re-reading and who were tested in the non-stressful way.

The authors go on to explain why stress inhibits learning and memory. In stressful situations, the brain releases cortisol which blocks the pathways to the hippocampus, the part of the brain largely responsible for memory. Retrieval practice can circumvents this, as the act of having to generate an answer creates numerous and clear routes to accessing the information.

The authors conclude that it's wise to teach students about the value of retrieval, be it multi-choice quizzes, self-tests, writing essays or verbally answering questions. They rather neatly sum this up by advising teachers to tell students: "Don't study in order to do well at the test; do lots of tests to study well."

Now, as language teachers often know well, acquiring a language is not the same as recalling factual information for tests, although it has elements in common. The research reminds me of the idea of "deep processing" when acquiring vocabulary - learn it in various ways (by listening, in chunks, from whole texts, with pictures, etc) and you are more likely to be able to recall it quickly in a stressful or non-stressful situation. In other words, that knowledge will be more secure and easily recalled than when you just learn words from a list or an app.

In any case, it's clear from the research that regular testing is a good thing.

I highly recommend the book to you. It's £14.77 on Amazon at the moment and would be a good investment for a school or department.




Comments

Popular posts from this blog

What is the natural order hypothesis?

The natural order hypothesis states that all learners acquire the grammatical structures of a language in roughly the same order. This applies to both first and second language acquisition. This order is not dependent on the ease with which a particular language feature can be taught; in English, some features, such as third-person "-s" ("he runs") are easy to teach in a classroom setting, but are not typically fully acquired until the later stages of language acquisition. The hypothesis was based on morpheme studies by Heidi Dulay and Marina Burt, which found that certain morphemes were predictably learned before others during the course of second language acquisition. The hypothesis was picked up by Stephen Krashen who incorporated it in his very well known input model of second language learning. Furthermore, according to the natural order hypothesis, the order of acquisition remains the same regardless of the teacher's explicit instruction; in other words,

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a language).

12 principles of second language teaching

This is a short, adapted extract from our book The Language Teacher Toolkit . "We could not possibly recommend a single overall method for second language teaching, but the growing body of research we now have points to certain provisional broad principles which might guide teachers. Canadian professors Patsy Lightbown and Nina Spada (2013), after reviewing a number of studies over the years to see whether it is better to just use meaning-based approaches or to include elements of explicit grammar teaching and practice, conclude: Classroom data from a number of studies offer support for the view that form-focused instruction and corrective feedback provided within the context of communicative and content-based programmes are more effective in promoting second language learning than programmes that are limited to a virtually exclusive emphasis on comprehension. As teachers Gianfranco and I would go along with that general view and would like to suggest our own set of g