Skip to main content

One way of teaching new grammatical patterns or vocabulary (via Boers, 2021)

 I’ve been reading and tweeting about the recently published book by Frank Boers called Evaluating Second Language Vocabulary and Grammar Instruction: a Synthesis of the Research on Teaching Words, Phrases and Patterns (published by Routledge).. I can recommend the book strongly to anyone interested in classroom second language acquisition research. It’s very clearly written, thorough and nuanced, notably in the way it evaluates research and draws tentative conclusions for the classroom.

In the final section of the book, Boers draws together the findings of research into incidental (by which he doesn’t mean unconscious, but more like picking up new language when the focus is on content or meaning), and language-focused learning and suggests one possible way to organise teaching if the aim is to introduce new items. It’s important to emphasise that point - this is when the precise aim is to develop skill with particular words, phrases or patterns.This is by no means meant to be prescriptive. As Boers puts it:

“The next question is how to create an ensemble of language-focused and content-focused activities to help language students (a) develop initial knowledge of a certain segment of vocabulary or of a certain grammar pattern, (b) retain this knowledge, and (c) use it smoothly for communicative purposes.”

He argues that because there are so many variables involved in classrooms, it’s “probably wise to adopt an eclectic approach”. But with that in mind, here is the sequence he describes which is evidenced by research:

1. Draw learners’ attention to a target words/phrases/pattern. 

2. Explain the meaning or function of the items/pattern with the aid of examples, or, if thought possible without a high risk of confusion, ask the learners to work them out themselves with the aid of examples, and then confirm.

3. Engage learners in content-oriented activities with input texts that further illustrate the use and meaning of the items/pattern. 

4. Possibly elaborate briefly about a property of the item that may make it easier to remember.

5. Provide opportunities for the students to retrieve the items/pattern from memory. This can be done in diverse ways. For example, a modified version of the input texts from step 3 could be used to cue recall of missing items (e.g. gap-fill). In the case of certain grammar features, it could be done through the kind of interpretation practice proposed in Processing Instruction (exercises which force students to make form-meaning links with the grammar, for example a verb ending). Recall of meaning can be prioritised if the aim is to encourage receptive knowledge; recall of form if the aim is to foster productive knowledge. 

6. Engage students in communicative tasks likely to elicit the target items/pattern. Ensure opportunities for improvement (e.g., feedback) between the tasks or task repetitions. Although there are more language-focused steps in this ensemble (steps 1, 2, 4 and 5), the content-focused activities (steps 3 and 6) would take up more time, thus creating a balance overall between the 2 broad approaches.

In this part of the book Boers references Nation’s (2007) ‘four strands’ approach to curriculum design, which includes focus on finding a balance between four aspects: meaning-focused input, meaning-focused output, a focus on the form of the language and fluency development. You can see that Boers’ own description of a typical sequence marries well with the Nation approach. As Boers also mentions, this sort of approach would hardly seem controversial to most teachers.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

What is the natural order hypothesis?

The natural order hypothesis states that all learners acquire the grammatical structures of a language in roughly the same order. This applies to both first and second language acquisition. This order is not dependent on the ease with which a particular language feature can be taught; in English, some features, such as third-person "-s" ("he runs") are easy to teach in a classroom setting, but are not typically fully acquired until the later stages of language acquisition. The hypothesis was based on morpheme studies by Heidi Dulay and Marina Burt, which found that certain morphemes were predictably learned before others during the course of second language acquisition. The hypothesis was picked up by Stephen Krashen who incorporated it in his very well known input model of second language learning. Furthermore, according to the natural order hypothesis, the order of acquisition remains the same regardless of the teacher's explicit instruction; in other words,

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a language).

12 principles of second language teaching

This is a short, adapted extract from our book The Language Teacher Toolkit . "We could not possibly recommend a single overall method for second language teaching, but the growing body of research we now have points to certain provisional broad principles which might guide teachers. Canadian professors Patsy Lightbown and Nina Spada (2013), after reviewing a number of studies over the years to see whether it is better to just use meaning-based approaches or to include elements of explicit grammar teaching and practice, conclude: Classroom data from a number of studies offer support for the view that form-focused instruction and corrective feedback provided within the context of communicative and content-based programmes are more effective in promoting second language learning than programmes that are limited to a virtually exclusive emphasis on comprehension. As teachers Gianfranco and I would go along with that general view and would like to suggest our own set of g