Skip to main content

Phonics bingo

The teaching of phonics (sound-spelling correspondences or SSCs), is definitely in vogue in England, not just for first language learners at primary school, but in modern language classrooms. For many years, teachers have taught the link between spelling and phonology, usually on an ad hoc basis. But since around 2016, when the TSC Review of pedagogy was published, the explicit teaching of phonics has been strongly recommended by the DfE via NCELP and, more recently, the Ofsted Research Review which itself quoted from the TSC Review (Bauckham, 2016). The NCELP resources not only embodied explicit phonics teaching, but a systematic approach to the same, i.e. planning out a sequence of when to introduce each SSC and design lessons partly based on that sequence.

The extent to which skill with phonics is acquired implicitly or through explicit teaching can be debated. Robert Woore (2022) looked at this and concluded that there is some, but not abundant, evidence that explicitly teaching sound-spelling correspondences can help students become better at spelling and pronouncing.

Whatever the evidence, the inspection body Ofsted expects to see evidence in classrooms and schemes of learning that explicit phonics teaching is going on. (The days are gone when Ofsted had no preference for teaching styles in classrooms, I think.)

My own belief is that phonics skill is primarily developed implicitly through classroom activities which involve seeing text while hearing it read aloud, reading aloud and the plethora of other classroom activities which involve listening, reading, speaking and writing. Explicit attention to the awkward areas (where the SSCs and phonology are most unlike the L1 system) can help a bit. Showing individual words with letters highlighted and choral repetition may induce boredom on its own.

In Breaking the Sound Barrier: Teaching Language Learners How to Listen (Conti and Smith, 2019) we provided many examples of activities and games which help develop phonics skill through use. One little game which builds sensitivity to sound and helps students match sounds to spelling is phonics bingo. Below is a resource I wrote for frenchteacher.ner which shows how the game may be played. The original Word doc is better formatted than below.

References

Bauckham. I. (2016). Available at: https://pure-research.york.ac.uk/ws/portalfiles/portal/54043904/MFL_Pedagogy_Review_Report_TSC_PUBLISHED_VERSION_Nov_2016_1_.pdf

Woore, R. (2022). What can second language acquisition research tell us about the phonics ‘pillar’? Available at: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/09571736.2022.2045683

**************************************************************************

1.    Hand out a bingo card to each student. There are eight provided. It doesn’t matter if two or three students get the same one. The key sound/spelling correspondences are IN BOLD on each card.

2.     Instead of reading out numbers, read out sounds (phonemes). Do not read out the same one more than once. So each game only last a couple of minutes.

3.     Students can swap cards for the next game, or you can hand out more cards.

 

Below are the sounds to be spoken. They are all vowel phonemes which can be spelled in different ways in French. I have not used accurate (IPA) phonetic alphabet symbols. Remember: just utter the sound, not the word.

/u/        As in ‘une’

/ou/      As in ‘vous’

/i/         As in ‘samedi’

/e/        As in ‘été’

/e:/       As in ‘mère’

/õ/        As in ‘ballon’

/in/       As in ‘main’

/un/      As in ‘brun’

/an/      As in ‘grand’

/eu/      As in ‘bleu’

/oi/       As in ‘moi’

/o/        As in ‘gros’

/ou/      As in ‘nous’

/a/        As in ‘animal’

 

Note that there are 14 sounds, but each card only contains 12. Warn the class that they may hear a sound which is not on their card at all.

Phonics bingo cards (8 different cards) – to be cut out and distributed, one to each pupil

 

chien

 

lundi

 

 

grand

 

petit

 

été

 

mère

 

gros

 

marron

 

une

 

bleu

 

nous

 

voiture

 

 

aime

 

brun

 

bateau

 

vous

 

chat

 

papa

 

brune

 

ballon

 

matin

 

aller

 

anglais

 

petits pois

 

 

vert

 

bien

 

lundi

 

cheveux

 

brune

 

toi

 

préfère

 

français

 

avons

 

Paris

 

main

 

coucou

 

 

enfant

 

moi

 

mon

 

vin

 

préfère

 

lundi

 

animal

 

musique

 

regarder

 

route

 

blond

 

souris

 

 

fin

 

lundi

 

 

blanc

 

petit

 

éléphant

 

aime

 

gros

 

poisson

 

lune

 

bleu

 

mouton

 

moi

 

 

aime

 

brun

 

eau

 

mouton

 

voiture

 

Irlande

 

brune

 

ballon

 

matin

 

écouter

 

grande

 

moi

 

 

frère

 

bien

 

lundi

 

sœur

 

brune

 

bois

 

préfère

 

cent

 

citron

 

pipi

 

main

 

loup

 

 

enfant

 

moi

 

mon

 

vin

 

préfère

 

lundi

 

fleur

 

musique

 

manger

 

route

 

allons

 

souris

 


Comments

Popular posts from this blog

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a language).

What is the natural order hypothesis?

The natural order hypothesis states that all learners acquire the grammatical structures of a language in roughly the same order. This applies to both first and second language acquisition. This order is not dependent on the ease with which a particular language feature can be taught; in English, some features, such as third-person "-s" ("he runs") are easy to teach in a classroom setting, but are not typically fully acquired until the later stages of language acquisition. The hypothesis was based on morpheme studies by Heidi Dulay and Marina Burt, which found that certain morphemes were predictably learned before others during the course of second language acquisition. The hypothesis was picked up by Stephen Krashen who incorporated it in his very well known input model of second language learning. Furthermore, according to the natural order hypothesis, the order of acquisition remains the same regardless of the teacher's explicit instruction; in other words,

12 principles of second language teaching

This is a short, adapted extract from our book The Language Teacher Toolkit . "We could not possibly recommend a single overall method for second language teaching, but the growing body of research we now have points to certain provisional broad principles which might guide teachers. Canadian professors Patsy Lightbown and Nina Spada (2013), after reviewing a number of studies over the years to see whether it is better to just use meaning-based approaches or to include elements of explicit grammar teaching and practice, conclude: Classroom data from a number of studies offer support for the view that form-focused instruction and corrective feedback provided within the context of communicative and content-based programmes are more effective in promoting second language learning than programmes that are limited to a virtually exclusive emphasis on comprehension. As teachers Gianfranco and I would go along with that general view and would like to suggest our own set of g