Skip to main content

Correcting students' spoken errors

In the run-up to the publication of the second edition of The Language Teacher Toolkit (Smith and Conti, 2023), I shall post the occasional blog post with short extracts from the text. This little section is about correcting correcting students' spoken errors. In the research literature this is usually referred to as corrective oral feedback. I previously posted something about 'learned attention', drawing on the work of Nick Ellis. This extract picks out some key research in this complex field of oral error correction. Elsewhere in the text we go into some detail about the value of correcting written errors.

******************************************************************************

A commonly asked question concerns the correction of students’ spoken errors. Should we do it? Which errors should we correct? How? When? The research in this area is copious and has, alas, produced mixed findings. Two terms are often used to describe how we correct oral errors: prompts and recasts. A prompt is an explicit correction of some type, e.g. where we correct the student or by the tone of our voice or facial expression, make it clear there was an error. A recast is where we give a correct reformulation of what a student said, without making it clear there was an error. (This distinction between prompts and recasts may not be clear-cut, since we cannot be sure if a student has grasped that the recast is actually a correction.)

Lyster and Ranta (1997) carried out an influential study of how teachers correct in the classroom and which types of correction work best. They found that recasts were the most common type of corrective feedback, but that students only occasionally ‘repaired’ (i.e. improved) their output as a response. It is pretty obvious why teachers like recasts, since they do not want to discourage students by correcting them. When students do repair their speech, the authors argue that this allows students to automatise the retrieval of L2 knowledge that already exists in some form. But also, when students repair, they draw on their own resources and confront errors which may lead to changes in their hypotheses about the L2.

In a nutshell, there is value in correction, done in the right way, at the right time, with the right students. Be aware that recasts may not be noticed, especially by less proficient students, and be selective with corrections, choosing errors which impede communication and students who may benefit from the correction. It is a subtle business!


Comments

Popular posts from this blog

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a language).

The 2026 GCSE subject content is published!

Two DfE documents were published today. The first was the response to the consultation about the proposed new GCSE (originally due in October 2021) and the second is the subject content document which, ultimately, is of most interest to MFL teachers in England. Here is the link  to the document.  We are talking about an exam to be done from 2026 (current Y7s). There is always a tendency for sceptical teachers to think that consultations are a bit of a sham and that the DfE will just go ahead and do what they want when it comes to exam reform. In this case, the responses to the original proposals were mixed, and most certainly hostile as far as exam boards and professional associations representing the MFL community, universities, head teachers and awarding bodies are concerned. What has emerged does reveal some significant changes which take account of a number of criticisms levelled at the proposals. As I read it, the most important changes relate to vocabulary and the issue of topics

La retraite à 60 ans

Suite à mon post récent sur les acquis sociaux..... L'âge légal de la retraite est une chose. Je voudrais bien savoir à quel âge les gens prennent leur retraite en pratique - l'âge réel de la retraite, si vous voulez. J'ai entendu prétendre qu'il y a peu de différence à cet égard entre la France et le Royaume-Uni. Manifestation à Marseille en 2008 pour le maintien de la retraite à 60 ans © AFP/Michel Gangne Six Français sur dix sont d’accord avec le PS qui défend la retraite à 60 ans (BVA) Cécile Quéguiner Plus de la moitié des Français jugent que le gouvernement a " tort de vouloir aller vite dans la réforme " et estiment que le PS a " raison de défendre l’âge légal de départ en retraite à 60 ans ". Résultat d’un sondage BVA/Absoluce pour Les Échos et France Info , paru ce matin. Une majorité de Français (58%) estiment que la position du Parti socialiste , qui défend le maintien de l’âge légal de départ à la retraite à 60 ans,