Skip to main content

Interaction in second language learning (Part 1)

This is the first of two posts summarising Chapter 6 of the book Second Language Learning Theories, by Mitchell, Myles and Marsden (Routledge, 3rd edition, 2013).

The authors begin by reminding us of the obvious fact using a second language is beneficial for learning it. Yet some people who are exposed for many years to a second language still make mistakes when using it. In the late 1970s Krashen hypothesised that receiving comprehensible input is all you need to acquire a language (the “Input Hypothesis”). Subsequently in the 1980s Michael Long, while accepting Stephen Krashen’s basic premiss and the existence of a “language acquisition device” in the brain (Chomsky), turned the focus towards interaction (the “Interaction Hypothesis”) suggesting that input alone is not enough and that interaction with the input is needed to learn.

Long argued that for learners to obtain input and repetition at the right level they need to take part in conversation. His early research indicated that during game-playing and conversation learners “negotiate meaning”, make clarification and confirmation requests, thus refining the input. In short, interaction makes the input more useful for acquisition.

More recent research from the 1990s has delved further into the role of interaction, focusing less on Krashen’s “black box” Language Acquisition Device and more on the latest findings from cognitive and language processing theory.
Some of this research has focused on the use of recasts and prompts during interaction.

If we step back a moment to the 1980s, complementing Long’s work Merrill Swain developed the “Output Hypothesis” (yes, second language acquisition research has many hypotheses!). Swain argued that learners’ own speech played a role in promoting noticing and intake of new language. We’ll return to this in Part 2.

Long’s 1996 revised Interaction Hypothesis

“It is proposed that environmental contributions to acquisition are mediated by selective attention and the learner’s developing L2 processing capacity, and that these resources are brought together most usefully, although not exclusively, during “negotiation for meaning”. Negative feedback obtained during negotiation work or elsewhere may be facilitative of L2 development, at least for vocabulary, morphology and language-specific syntax, and essential for learning certain specifiable contrasts.” (Long, 1996)

We should understand from the last part of that quotation that Long believes some aspects of a language need particular attention and feedback, e.g. correction, to be mastered. Long’s hypothesis bears in mind the mental processes involved in getting INPUT to become INTAKE. These include the use of selective attention and L2 processing capacity.

Interaction and the learning of target L2 structures and vocabulary

Mackey (1999) carried out a study to find out if interaction would boost the knowledge of question forms on English as a second language. The low-intermediate adult learners did a range of information gap tasks which required them to ask questions. Some participants in the study had the opportunity to interact with the teacher while were not. His results indicated that the “interactors” progressed with question formation while the others didn’t. Other studies have confirmed that interaction assists with the acquisition of specific aspects of language.

The role of feedback

The role of feedback on learners’ errors has long been a focus of research, even before the interest in interactionist theory. Those who support the Universal Grammar view of things, e.g. Krashen, argue that correction plays an unimportant role. Learners develop proficiency and accuracy through input. However Long (1996) argues that negative feedback can be useful to learners. By this he means that feedback involving reformulation of the learner’s own intended utterances can help the learner notice the gaps between their performance and the correct version. For example, learners may become aware when there are differences between the L1 and L2 structure.

Separately, researchers who see second language learning as a form of skill acquisition (i.e. language learning is like the learning of any complex skill), believe feedback helps with the establishment and proceduralisation of declarative knowledge. In other words correction can help with the automatisation of linguistic skill.

Research on types of error correction, e.g. recasts and explicit correction have produced mixed results. In general the research suggests that corrective feedback tends to be useful, but that no particular type has been shown to be better than others. The authors note, by the way, that recasts can be seen not so much as corrective feedback but just another source of correct input, depending on how much attention the learner is paying. Recasts can be seen therefore as positive reinforcement. In contrast, prompts of explicit correction may be less ambiguous and therefore moe useful to learners.

Part two to follow.

References

Long, M. H. (1996). The role of linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In Ritchie and Bhatia (eds.) Handbook of second language acquisition. San Diego: Academic Press.

Mackey, A. (1999). Input, interaction and second language development: an empirical study of question formation in ESL. Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

Mitchell, R., Myles, F. and Marsden, E. (2013). Second Language Learning Theories. Oxford: Routledge.



- Posted using BlogPress from my iPad

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

What is the natural order hypothesis?

The natural order hypothesis states that all learners acquire the grammatical structures of a language in roughly the same order. This applies to both first and second language acquisition. This order is not dependent on the ease with which a particular language feature can be taught; in English, some features, such as third-person "-s" ("he runs") are easy to teach in a classroom setting, but are not typically fully acquired until the later stages of language acquisition. The hypothesis was based on morpheme studies by Heidi Dulay and Marina Burt, which found that certain morphemes were predictably learned before others during the course of second language acquisition. The hypothesis was picked up by Stephen Krashen who incorporated it in his very well known input model of second language learning. Furthermore, according to the natural order hypothesis, the order of acquisition remains the same regardless of the teacher's explicit instruction; in other words,

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a language).

12 principles of second language teaching

This is a short, adapted extract from our book The Language Teacher Toolkit . "We could not possibly recommend a single overall method for second language teaching, but the growing body of research we now have points to certain provisional broad principles which might guide teachers. Canadian professors Patsy Lightbown and Nina Spada (2013), after reviewing a number of studies over the years to see whether it is better to just use meaning-based approaches or to include elements of explicit grammar teaching and practice, conclude: Classroom data from a number of studies offer support for the view that form-focused instruction and corrective feedback provided within the context of communicative and content-based programmes are more effective in promoting second language learning than programmes that are limited to a virtually exclusive emphasis on comprehension. As teachers Gianfranco and I would go along with that general view and would like to suggest our own set of g