Skip to main content

What will the new GCSE speaking test look like in 2025?

 As you probably know, there is a consultation taking place at the moment about the proposals for the revised GCSE (first teaching September 2023, first exams June 2025). I have blogged about this a few times before, but in this post I'd like to look at the proposal for the speaking assessment.

The key paragraph relating to this, from the subject content document (p.3) states that students will have to:

  1. Read aloud, using clear and comprehensible pronunciation, short sentences from the written form of the language and demonstrate understanding of them (for example by answering questions); 
  2. Undertake role play simulating a context such as a social conversation where instructions are unambiguous and there are no unexpected questions; 
  3. Answer questions about a visual stimulus such as a photograph. 

For these activities students will have time for preparation (without access to reference materials), and vocabulary and grammar expected will be from the defined content for each tier.

Here is the key document:

MFL GCSE Subject Content (education.gov.uk)

This looks clear enough, so we might envisage a few sentences or short paragraph to read aloud, followed by, say, three questions. This might last around a minute or so, perhaps longer for Higher Tier.

The role play (possibly more than one) with prompts (in English) and no unexpected questions. This might take a minute or so. Higher Tier candidates might get more challenging prompts.

A stimulus card, most likely a photo, or maybe a photo or graphic with a little text. In this case, it looks like there will be no questions provided in advance, so students would have to respond spontaneously. This looks like it approaches what we are familiar with in terms of general conversation. It is not at all clear what the subject matter might be or how the questioning would be structured. This might last around 5 minutes at Higher Tier or 3 minutes at Foundation Tier.

One criticism that has been levelled at the proposal is that it devalues the role of spontaneous conversation. I am not yet clear that this is true. As I read it, the photo/stimulus section (part 3) does have the potential to allow the strongest candidates to take the conversation in directions they would like, possibly introducing elements of pre-learned material (nothing wrong with that up to a point). But we need to see what the awarding bodies (exam boards) come up with, or are allowed to come up with.

I welcome the fact that the role-play looks like it will resemble a social conversation rather rather than an even more contrived transactional situation of the type "You are in a clothes shop...". I also welcome the fact that prompts will be in English. The current format with its L2 prompts, imposed by Ofqual and not the preference of exam boards, is confusing.

Arguably the most controversial element is the reading aloud element, which I have blogged about before. In that post, I argued that, on balance, you could make a case for including reading aloud if the 'backwash effect' is positive, i.e. if it encourages teachers to do more reading aloud in class. My friend Gianfranco Conti has come to the view that we don't need a reading aloud test in Y11 to encourage teachers to do reading aloud at KS3 (age 11.14). He made an appealing analogy that you don't assess a footballer's skill by testing if they can pass. Reading aloud should be sorted out by the end of Y9 at the latest. I am tempted by this perspective, but I confess that my feelings are still mixed.

A fundamental question is this: should it be the role of DfE/Ofqual/GCSE to push teachers towards specific methodologies, techniques and classroom procedures? Let's be clear that to do so would not be new. Previously, translation was added to the roster of GCSE question types with the clear intention of pushing teachers to do more grammar and translation. Mixed skill questions, combining L2 reading with L2 speaking for example, have been included partly to encourage such practice in the classroom. In 1987-88, when GCSE replaced O-level and CSE, the question types were designed to align with a more communicative approach to teaching, with its greater emphasis on listening and speaking. Going back to O-level itself, the inclusion of translation, dictation, multi-choice comprehension, and picture sequence compositions meant that these were practised a lot in class.

No exam can be be said to be "pedagogically neutral" since, in our system, with GCSE being such a high-stakes exam, the exam format will encourage teachers to teach to the test. The DfE want teachers to do more grammar, more phonics and more dictation, so they are pushing for an exam with that in mind. They favour the type of syllabus promoted by NCELP which is based on a structural grammar syllabus (often criticised in the research literature), high-frequency vocabulary (which gets more support) and phonics (for which there is actually little evidence from longitudinal studies, but can easily be justified by other research). The extent to which you like the proposals will reflect your view of NCELP's principles.

As for the oral, let's see what the consultation produces and what exam boards come up with.

Comments, as always, are welcome.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

What is the natural order hypothesis?

The natural order hypothesis states that all learners acquire the grammatical structures of a language in roughly the same order. This applies to both first and second language acquisition. This order is not dependent on the ease with which a particular language feature can be taught; in English, some features, such as third-person "-s" ("he runs") are easy to teach in a classroom setting, but are not typically fully acquired until the later stages of language acquisition. The hypothesis was based on morpheme studies by Heidi Dulay and Marina Burt, which found that certain morphemes were predictably learned before others during the course of second language acquisition. The hypothesis was picked up by Stephen Krashen who incorporated it in his very well known input model of second language learning. Furthermore, according to the natural order hypothesis, the order of acquisition remains the same regardless of the teacher's explicit instruction; in other words,

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a language).

12 principles of second language teaching

This is a short, adapted extract from our book The Language Teacher Toolkit . "We could not possibly recommend a single overall method for second language teaching, but the growing body of research we now have points to certain provisional broad principles which might guide teachers. Canadian professors Patsy Lightbown and Nina Spada (2013), after reviewing a number of studies over the years to see whether it is better to just use meaning-based approaches or to include elements of explicit grammar teaching and practice, conclude: Classroom data from a number of studies offer support for the view that form-focused instruction and corrective feedback provided within the context of communicative and content-based programmes are more effective in promoting second language learning than programmes that are limited to a virtually exclusive emphasis on comprehension. As teachers Gianfranco and I would go along with that general view and would like to suggest our own set of g