Skip to main content

How checklists can raise student proficiency

I am grateful to Martin Heeley, a PGCE student at York University for reminding me about how useful checklists can be in improving students' performance on tasks and I shall use an example of his below to demonstrate the point.

Example one: for advanced level conversation practice

When I used to prepare A-level students for their AQA oral examination which included 10 minutes of conversation on the two cultural topics they had been studying, in the run-up to the exam I would pair off students to practise questions I had provided them with. I would periodically change the pairings to keep up interest and provide a slightly different focus. To improve their range of language I would then write up a checklist of expressions they had to include in their conversations. So, let's say they were preparing for a discussion on the films of François Truffaut, I might write up:

Un élément clé de la nouvelle vague...
Ce qui est important, c'est le fait que...
Il va sans dire que...
Les opinions sont partagées sur cette question, mais moi...
En ce qui concerne l'usage de la caméra, on peut dire que...
On ne peut pas sous-estimer l'influence de la vie de Truffaut sur...

I would then say that in the next five minutes, the "candidate" had to incorporate somehow each of those expressions into their conversation. This would become a source of challenge and amusement, focus minds a bit more and gradually the expressions would become handy elements to be used in the future. Individual students would take pleasure in deliberately overusing certain phrases for comic effect. Needless to say, such expressions become handy comfort blankets in the actual exam.

Five minutes later I would alter the list to widen the students' range of expression, perhaps giving my own examples of how the formulae could be used.

Example two: GCSE/intermediate level writing

The principle is just the same as the above, except this time you provide a set of phrases or expressions which students have to incorporate into a written composition. So, let's say students had to describe a journey they had made, you might give them:

Apès être parti(s) de la maison...
En arrivant à....
Ce qui m'a plu le plus, c'était...
Le pire moment était quand...
On venait de + infinitif.... quand...
Le plus intéressant, c'était le moment où...
Avant de + infinitif

We know what happens when you adopt this approach. Just as in the A-level example, students latch on to these lifebuoys and use them in subsequent work, gradually increasing their repertoire of language.


Example three: Martin Heeley's example (via http://mrheeley.wordpress.com/2014/06/15/check/)

This is another writing example at a post beginner/very low intermediate level. I like the fact that this type of checklist makes pupils reflect on what it takes to write interesting and varied language. They do a lot of this type of thing in English lessons and it really helps pupils who are genuinely not sure what to write.


As examination boards move back to linear papers and away from controlled assessment, students will need an armoury of phrases and little techniques to help them write and speak under pressure. Teachers would do well to make good use of checklists.


Comments

  1. Thanks for this.

    When mine are preparing for assessment, I make them generate their own checklist based on what I told them after their last assessment: http://alexfoster.me.uk/2014/06/15/checklists-in-mfl-building-on-marking/

    ReplyDelete
  2. Thanks for commenting. I have added your blog to my blog roll on frenchteacher.net.

    ReplyDelete

Post a Comment

Popular posts from this blog

What is the natural order hypothesis?

The natural order hypothesis states that all learners acquire the grammatical structures of a language in roughly the same order. This applies to both first and second language acquisition. This order is not dependent on the ease with which a particular language feature can be taught; in English, some features, such as third-person "-s" ("he runs") are easy to teach in a classroom setting, but are not typically fully acquired until the later stages of language acquisition. The hypothesis was based on morpheme studies by Heidi Dulay and Marina Burt, which found that certain morphemes were predictably learned before others during the course of second language acquisition. The hypothesis was picked up by Stephen Krashen who incorporated it in his very well known input model of second language learning. Furthermore, according to the natural order hypothesis, the order of acquisition remains the same regardless of the teacher's explicit instruction; in other words,

Second language learning and acquisition

This is a long, referenced blog which combines all the posts in my earlier series entitled Conscious and Unconscious Language Learning. If you have already read those posts, you should look away now. Part 1 Throughout the history of the study of language learning and teaching reference has been made to two distinct types of language learning. The first could be characterised as "picking up" a language and normally involves the apparently unconscious acquisition of a language in an informal or natural setting. One thinks of the child who learns their native tongue, or the immigrant who learns the new language without recourse to formal study. The second type of language learning involves the practice of a language in a formal, systematic way, often in a classroom setting. This has frequently been termed conscious learning. Such a clear distinction may be controversial and you may already be thinking, quite reasonably, that both types of learning have a role. However, when

What is "Input Processing"?

Input Processing (IP) was proposed by Bill VanPatten, Professor of Spanish and Second Language Acquisition from Michigan State University. Bill may be known to some of you from his podcast show Tea with BVP. He is one of those rare university academics who makes a specific effort to engage with practising teachers. IP was first proposed in a 1993 article (published with T. Cadierno in the Modern Language Journal) entitled "Input processing and second language acquisition: A role for instruction." My summary of it is based on an article "Input Processing and Processing Instruction: Definitions and Issues" (2013) by Hossein Hashemnezhad. IP is a little complicated to explain, but I'll do my best to summarise the key points before suggesting how it relates to other ways of looking at classroom language teaching. Is this actually any use to teachers? I apologise in advance for over-simplifying or misunderstanding. To paraphrase Dr Leonard McCoy from Star Trek &q

New MFL GCSE consultation

Updated on 7th April, with a few modifications to the original post written about a month earlier. ........................................................................... The DfE in England has recently published information about the proposed new GCSE exams, first teaching September 2023, first exams June 2025. There are two consultations going on, one regarding the subject content, and the other (much shorter) with respect to the assessment arrangements such as tiering.  The context is important here. DfE are worried about uptake in GCSE MFL, especially with their EBacc target of 90% uptake in mind. (This is highly unlikely to be achieved.) Therefore they would like an exam which makes the subject more attractive, both in terms of interesting content and accessibility (how easy it is thought to be). They are aware also of criticisms levelled at current papers that the exam is elitist, featuring too much subject matter which appeals to middle class students. Recall that MFL has be

Pros and cons of pair and group work

Most teachers have made frequent use of pair and group work for many years, notably since the rise of communicative language teaching in the 1980s. Even before then it would have been common for pupils to work in pairs on simple role-play and dialogue tasks. So pair and group work is standard practice, if not universally supported by language teachers. It’s always worth evaluating, however, whether a practice works - whether, in this case, it helps students develop their proficiency. Pros Rod Ellis (2005) summarises the advantages of pair/group work (based on Jacobs, 1998) “1. The quantity of learner speech can increase. In teacher-fronted classrooms, the teacher typically speaks 80% of the time; in groupwork more students talk for more of the time. 2. The variety of speech acts can increase. In teacher-fronted classrooms, students are cast in a responsive role, but in groupwork they can perform a wide range of roles, including those involved in the negotiation of meaning. 3. There can